Keres language

(Redirected from Keresan languages)

Keres (/ˈkrs/),[2] also Keresan (/ˈkɛrəsən/), is a Native American language, spoken by the Keres Pueblo people in New Mexico. Depending on the analysis, Keres is considered a small language family or a language isolate with several dialects. If it is considered a language isolate, it would be the most widely spoken language isolate within the borders of the United States. The varieties of each of the seven Keres pueblos are mutually intelligible with its closest neighbors. There are significant differences between the Western and Eastern groups, which are sometimes counted as separate languages.

Keresan
Native toUnited States
RegionNew Mexico
EthnicityKeres
Native speakers
13,190 (2013)[1]
Dialects
  • East Keres
  • West Keres
Language codes
ISO 639-3Either:
kee – Eastern
kjq – Western
Glottologkere1287
ELP
Pre-contact distribution of Keresan languages
Acoma-Laguna is classified as Definitely Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger

Classification

edit

Keres is now considered a language isolate. In the past, Edward Sapir grouped it together with a Hokan–Siouan stock. Morris Swadesh suggested a connection with Wichita. Joseph Greenberg grouped Keres with Siouan, Yuchi, Caddoan, and Iroquoian in a superstock called Keresiouan. None of these proposals has been validated by subsequent linguistic research.

Internal classification

edit

In 2007, there was an estimate total of 10,670 speakers.[3]

  • Keres
    • Eastern Keres: total of 4,580 speakers (1990 census)
    • Western Keres: total of 3,391 speakers (1990 census)

Phonology

edit

Keresan has between 42 and 45 consonant sounds, and around 40 vowel sounds, adding up to a total of about 85 phonemes, depending on the analysis and the language variety. Based on the classification in the World Atlas of Language Structures, Keres is a language with a large consonant inventory.

The great number of consonants relates to the three-way distinction between voiceless, aspirated and ejective consonants (e.g. /t tʰ tʼ/), and to the larger than average[4] number of fricatives (i.e. /s sʼ ʂ ʂʼ ʃ ʃʼ h/) and affricates, the latter also showing the three-way distinction found in stops.

The large number of vowels derives from a distinction made between long and short vowels (e.g. /e eː/), as well as from the presence of tones and voicelessness. Thus, a single vowel quality may occur with seven distinct realizations: /é è e̥ éː èː êː ěː/, all of which are used to distinguish words in the language.

Consonants

edit

The chart below contains the consonants of the proto-Keresan (or pre-Keresan) from Miller & Davis (1963) based on a comparison of Acoma, Santa Ana, and Santo Domingo, as well as other features of the dialects compiled from The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964), Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987), and The Phonemes of Keresan (1946), and the Grammar of Laguna Keres (2005).[5][6][7][8]

Labial Alveolar Palatal Retroflex Velar Glottal
Nasal voiced m n ɲ
glottalized ɲˀ
Plosive voiceless p t c k ʔ
aspirated
ejective
Affricate voiceless ts
aspirated tsʰ tʃʰ tʂʰ
ejective tsʼ tʃʼ tʂʼ
Fricative voiceless s ʃ ʂ h
ejective ʃʼ ʂʼ
Approximant voiced w ɾ j
glottalized ɾˀ

Vowels

edit

Keresan vowels have a phonemic distinction in duration: all vowels can be long or short. Additionally, short vowels can also be voiceless. The vowel chart below contains the vowel phonemes and allophones from the information of the Keresan languages combined from The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo (1964),[5] The Phonemes of Keresan (1946),[7] and Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics (1987).[6]

Long Short
Phonemic Phonetic Phonemic Phonetic Voiceless
Close /iː/ [i] /i/ [i ɪ] [ɪ̥]
Mid-front /eː/ [eː] /e/ [e ɛ æ] [e̥]
Mid-central /ɨː/ [əː ɨː] /ɨ/ [ə ɨ ɤ] [ɨ̥]
Open /ɑː/ [aː ɑː] /ɑ/ [a ɑ] [ḁ]
Back-close /oː/ [oː] /o/ [o] [o̥]
/uː/ [uː] /u/ [u ʊ o] [ʊ̥]

Notes:

  • Western Keres does not have phonemic /oː/ or /o/, though both vowels may occur phonetically.[8] Eastern Keres words containing /o/ show /au/ in Western Keres.[9] For instance, the first vowel in the word-sentence Sraúka̠cha̠ – “I see you”:
    • Kotyit Keres: [ʂóːkʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]
    • Kʼawaika Keres: [ʂɑ̌ukʰɑ̥tʃʰɑ̥]

Voiceless vowels

edit

All Keresan short vowels may be devoiced in certain positions. The phonemic status of these vowels is controversial.[8] Maring (1967) considers them to be phonemes of Áákʼu Keres, whereas other authors disagree. There are phonetic grounds for vowel devoicing based on the environment they occur, for instance word-finally, but there are also exceptions. Vowels in final position are nearly always voiceless and medial vowels occurring between voiced consonants, after nasals and ejectives are nearly always voiced.[10]

  • Word-final devoicing: [pɑ̌ːkʊ̥] because
  • Word-medial devoicing: [ʔìpʰi̥ʃɑ́] white paint

Tones

edit

Acoma Keres has four lexical tones: high, low, falling and rising.[10] Falling and rising tones only occur in long vowels and voiceless vowels bear no tones:

Tones examples translation
High [tɨ́j], [áwáʔáwá] here, matrilineal uncle
Low [mùːtètsá] young boy
Rising [pɑ̌ːkʊ̥] because
Falling [ʔêː], [hêːk'a] and, whole part

Syllable structure

edit

Most Keresan syllables take a CV(V) shape.[8] The maximal syllable structure is CCVVC and the minimal syllable is CV. In native Keresan words, only a glottal stop /ʔ/ ⟨ʼ⟩ can close a syllable, but some loanwords from Spanish have syllables that end in a consonant, mostly a nasal (i.e. /m n/ but words containing these sequences are rare in the language.[11]

Syllable type examples translation
CV [sʼà], [ʔɪ]shv́v I have it, left
CVV [mùː]dedza, a[táù]shi young boy, cooking pot
CCV [ʃkʰí]srátsʼa I'm not fat
CCVV [ʃtùː]sra bluejay
CVC í[miʔ], [kùm]banêeru expression of fear, workmate (Spanish "compañero")

Due to extensive vowel devoicing, several Keresan words may be perceived as ending in consonants or even containing consonant clusters.

  • Word-internal cluster: yʼâakạ srûunị ‘stomach’ /jˀɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni/ > [jɑ̂ːkḁʂûːni] ~ [jɑ̂ːûːni]
  • Word-final coda: úwàakạ ‘baby’; /úwɑ̀ːkḁ/ > [úwɑ̀ːkʰḁ] ~ [úwɑ̀ː]

Phonotactics

edit

The only sequence of consonants (i.e. consonant cluster) that occurs in native Keresan words is a sequence of a fricative /ʃ ʂ/ and a stop or affricate. Clusters are restricted to beginnings of syllables (i.e. the syllable onset). When the alveolo-palatal consonant /ʃ/ occurs as C1, it combines with alveolar and palatal C2, whereas the retroflex alveolar /ʂ/ precedes bilabial and velar C2s, which suggest a complementary distribution. Consonant clusters may occur both word-initially and word-medially.[9]

C1/C2 Bilabial Alveolar Velar Postalveolar
/p/ /pʰ/ /pʼ/ /t/ /tʰ/ /tʼ/ /k/ /kʰ/ /kʼ/ /tʃ/ /tʃʰ/ /tʃʼ/
/ʃ/ /ʃtáʊ̯rákʊ̥/

shdáurákụ

'frog, toad'

/ʃtʰéràʃtʼíká/

shtérashtʼígá

'cricket'

/ʃtʼìcɑ̀ːtʰɪ̥ʃɪ̥/

shtʼidyàatịshị

'plot of land'

/ʃtʃɨ/

shjv

'upward'

/ʃtʃʰúmúná/

shchúmúmá

'wasp'

/ʃtʃʼísḁ/

shchʼísạ

'six'

/ʂ/ /ʂpúːná/

srbúuná

'water jug'

/ʂpʰɑ̀ːtʼi/

srpàat'i

'mockingbird'

/ʂpʼeruru/

srpʼeruru

'it's full'

/ʂkɑ́ʂkɑ́ʊ̯kʼa/

srgásrgáukʼa

'quail'

/ʂkʰɨ́tútsʰɪ̥/

srkv́dútsị

'mound, hill'

/ʂkʼàpɪ́hɪ́/

srkʼabíhí

'female in-law'

Orthography

edit

Traditional Keresan beliefs postulate that Keres is a sacred language that must exist only in its spoken form.[12] The language's religious connotation and years of persecution of Pueblo religion by European colonizers may also explain why no unified orthographic convention exists for Keresan. However, a practical spelling system has been developed for Laguna (Kʼawaika)[8] and more recently for Acoma (Áakʼu) Keres,[13] both of which are remarkably consistent.

In the Keres spelling system, each symbol represents a single phoneme. The letters ⟨c q z f⟩ and sometimes also ⟨v⟩ are not used. Digraphs represent both palatal consonants (written using a sequence of C and ⟨y⟩), and retroflex consonants, which are represented using a sequence of C and the letter ⟨r⟩. These graphemes used for writing Western Keres are shown between ⟨...⟩ below.

Consonant symbols

edit
Labial Alveolar Palatal Retroflex Velar Glottal
Nasal voiced ⟨m⟩ ⟨n⟩ ⟨ny⟩
glottalized ⟨mʼ⟩ ⟨nʼ⟩ ⟨nyʼ⟩
Plosive voiceless ⟨b⟩ ⟨d⟩ ⟨dy⟩ ⟨g⟩ ⟨ʼ⟩
aspirated ⟨p⟩ ⟨t⟩ ⟨ty⟩ ⟨k⟩
ejective ⟨pʼ⟩ ⟨tʼ⟩ ⟨tyʼ⟩ ⟨kʼ⟩
Affricate voiceless ⟨dz⟩ ⟨j⟩ ⟨dr⟩
aspirated ⟨ts⟩ ⟨ch⟩ ⟨tr⟩
ejective ⟨tsʼ⟩ ⟨chʼ⟩ ⟨trʼ⟩
Fricative voiceless ⟨s⟩ ⟨sh⟩ ⟨sr⟩ ⟨h⟩
ejective ⟨sʼ⟩ ⟨shʼ⟩ ⟨srʼ⟩
Approximant voiced ⟨w⟩ ⟨r⟩ ⟨y⟩
glottalized ⟨wʼ⟩ ⟨rʼ⟩ ⟨yʼ⟩

Signage at Acoma Pueblo

edit

Signs at Acoma Pueblo sometimes use special diacritics for ejective consonants that differ from the symbols above, as shown in the table:

 
Signage at Acoma Pueblo
Comparison between general orthpgraphy and the orthography used at Acoma
General ⟨pʼ⟩ ⟨tʼ⟩ ⟨kʼ⟩ ⟨sʼ⟩ ⟨tsʼ⟩ ⟨mʼ⟩ ⟨wʼ⟩ ⟨yʼ⟩ ⟨nʼ shʼ srʼ tyʼ⟩
Acoma signage ⟨ṕ⟩ ⟨t́⟩ ⟨ḱ⟩ ⟨ś⟩ ⟨tś⟩ ⟨ḿ⟩ ⟨ẃ⟩ ⟨ý⟩ ?


Vowel symbols

edit

Vowel sounds are represented straightforwardly in the existing spellings for Keresan. Each vowel sound is written using a unique letter or digraph (for long vowels and diphthongs). However, there are two competing representations for the vowel /ɨ/. Some versions simply use the IPA ⟨ɨ⟩ whereas others use the letter ⟨v⟩ (the sound /v/ as in veal does not occur in Keresan). Voiceless vowels have also been represented in two ways; either underlined or with a dot below (see table).

Long vowels Short vowels Voiceless vowels
Phoneme Grapheme Phoneme Grapheme Phoneme Grapheme
/ iː / ⟨ii⟩ / i / ⟨i⟩ / ɪ̥ / ⟨i̱⟩ or ⟨ị⟩
/ eː / ⟨ee⟩ / e / ⟨e⟩ / e̥ / ⟨e̱⟩ or ⟨ẹ⟩
/ ɨː / ⟨ɨɨ⟩ or ⟨vv⟩ / ɨ / ⟨ɨ⟩ or ⟨v⟩ / ɨ̥ / ⟨ɨ̱⟩ or ⟨ṿ⟩
/ ɑː / ⟨aa⟩ / ɑ / ⟨a⟩ / ḁ / ⟨a̱⟩ or ⟨ạ⟩
/ oː / ⟨oo⟩ / o / ⟨o⟩ / o̥ / ⟨o̱⟩ or ⟨ọ⟩
/ uː / ⟨uu⟩ / u / ⟨u⟩ / ʊ̥ / ⟨u̱⟩ or ⟨ụ⟩

Diacritics for tone

edit

Tone may or may not be represented in the orthography of Keresan. When represented, four diacritics may be used above the vowel. Unlike the system used for Navajo, diacritics for tone are not repeated in long vowels.

High tone Low tone Rising tone Falling tone
Long Vowel ⟨áa⟩, ⟨úu⟩ ⟨àa⟩, ⟨ùu⟩ or unmarked ⟨ǎa⟩, ⟨ǔu⟩ or ⟨aá⟩, ⟨uú⟩ ⟨âa⟩, ⟨ûu⟩ or ⟨aà⟩, ⟨uù⟩
Short Vowel ⟨á⟩, ⟨ú⟩ ⟨à⟩, ⟨ù⟩ or unmarked -

Keres orthography and alphabetical order

edit

Although Keresan is not normally written, there exists one dictionary of the language in which words are listed in any given order. In this dictionary of Western Keres, digraphs count as single letters, although ejective consonants are not listed separately; occurring after their non-ejective counterparts. The symbol for the glottal stop ⟨ʼ⟩, for long vowels (e.g. ⟨aa ee ii⟩ etc.) are not treated as separate letters.

Alphabetical order in the Acoma Keres Audio Dictionary
A a B b CH ch CHʼ chʼ D d DR dr DY dy DZ dz E e G g H h I i (Ɨ ɨ) J j K k Kʼ kʼ
M m Mʼ mʼ N n Nʼ nʼ NY ny NYʼ nyʼ (O o) P p Pʼ pʼ R r Rʼ rʼ S s Sʼ sʼ SH sh SHʼ shʼ SR sr
SRʼ srʼ T tʼ TR tr TRʼ trʼ TS ts TSʼ tsʼ TY ty TYʼ tyʼ U u W w (V v) Wʼ wʼ Y y Yʼ yʼ

Letters〈f q x z〉are not used to write Keres, whereas the letters ⟨ɨ o v⟩ are only used in some dialects.


Sample texts

edit

Orthography marking tone

edit
Woodpecker and Coyote[9]
⟨Ái dítʼîishu srbígà kʼánâaya dyáʼâʼu. Shʼée srbígà ái dyěitsị ái náyáa shdyɨ dyáʼa.⟩
/ ɑ́ì títʼîːʃù ʂpíkɑ̀ kʼɑ́nɑ̂ːjɑ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̂ʔù | ʃʼéː ʂpíkɑ̀ ɑ́ì cěǐtsʰi̥ ɑ́ì nɑ́jɑ́ː ʃcɨ̀ cɑ́ʔɑ̀ /

Orthography without tone marking

edit
Boas text[8]
Baanaʼa, egu kauʼseeʼe, atsi sʼaama-ee srayutse.

Morphosyntax

edit

Keresan is a split-ergative language in which verbs denoting states (i.e. stative verbs) behave differently from those indexing actions, especially in terms of the person affixes they take. This system of argument marking is based on a split-intransitive pattern, in which subjects are marked differently if they are perceived as actors than from when they are perceived as undergoers of the action being described.

The morphology of Keresan is mostly prefixing, although suffixes and reduplication also occur.[9] Keresan distinguishes nouns, verbs, numerals and particles as word classes. Nouns in Keresan do not normally distinguish case or number, but they can be inflected for possession, with distinct constructions for alienable and inalienable possession. Other than possession, Keresan nouns show no comprehensive noun classes.

Word order

edit

Keresan is a verb-final language, though word order is rather flexible.[9][8]

Laguna Keres:[8]

John

J.

John

subject

Bill

B.

Bill

object

gukacha

g-Ø-ukacha

3S-3S-see

verb

John Bill gukacha

J. B. g-Ø-ukacha

John Bill 3S-3S-see

subject object verb

'John saw Bill.'

Negation

edit

Negation is doubly marked in Keresan. In addition to the adverb dzaadi, verbs index negation through a suffix (e.g. -u).

  • Gukacha 'S/he saw her/him'
  • Dzaadi gukachau 'S/he didn't see her/him'

Verbal morphology

edit

The verb is a central grammatical category in Keres, conveying the most information about events in communicative acts.[8][9][10] Through its morphemes, Keresan verbs code not only person and number of the initiator of the action (e.g. “Tammy drinks decaf”) as is common in Indo-European languages, but also how the initiator is implicated in the action. For instance, the three verbs that describe Tammy's actions in “Tammy kicked the ball” vs. “Tammy jumped” vs. “Tammy sneezed” require different levels of effort from Tammy, that is when kicking vs. jumping vs. sneezing.

Additionally, the person and number of the undergoer of the action are all coded on the verb (e.g. the word gukacha means “S/he sees her/him”, a full sentence in English). The ways the speaker assesses the action (i.e. evidentiality, as in “I think Tammy arrived from class” vs. “Tammy arrived from class”). Finally, the internal temporal structure of the action (i.e. aspect, as in “Tammy was sneezing in class” vs. “Tammy sneezed in class”) is also coded in Keresan verbs.

According to Maring (1967), the Keresan verb is organized around the following grammatical categories (pp. 39–40)[10]

  • Subject/Object relations
    • Subject of intransitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Subject of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that distinguishes 3-4 persons in the singular (see below).
    • Object of transitive verbs: marked by a prefix that combines with the subject prefix, or by a suffix
  • Number relations
    • Singular: usually marked by a prefix
    • Dual: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication or by suffixes
    • Plural: can be marked by a prefix, partial reduplication, by suffixes or by suppletive stem forms (i.e. singular and plural forms are not related etymologically)
  • Temporal relations
    • Future: is marked on the verb by a series of prefixes that also encode number
  • Modality relations
  • Voice relations
  • Aspect
    • Imperfective
    • Inceptive
    • Repetitive
    • Continuative
    • Habitual
    • Inchoative
    • Perfective

The verbal prefix

edit

In Keres, the verbal prefix carries information from five different grammatical categories: argument role, modality, polarity,[8] person and number. That is, a single Keresan verb prefix codes who initiated the action and how implicated that entity is (the subject/case), whom underwent the effects of the action (the direct object), the speaker's assessment of the action (the modality)[14] and whether it occurred or not (polarity). On the other hand, information about when the action took place (i.e. tense) is expressed elsewhere in a clause, mostly by adverbs.[9]

Number
edit

Keresan verbs distinguish three numbers: singular, dual (two entities) and plural (more than two entities); and four persons: first (the speaker), second (the hearer), third (a known, definite or salient entity being talked about) and fourth (a non-salient, unknown or indefinite entity being talked about, also known as obviative) persons. The plural and dual forms are often marked by reduplication of part of the stem (gukacha ‘s/he saw it’ vs guʼukacha ‘the two of them saw it’).

Argument role
edit

Languages encode two main types of actions: those in which the main participant initiates an action that produces change in an object (e.g. kick a ball, buy a gift, cook a dish, read a book); and those in which the action produces no (perceived) change in the world or that have no object (sneezing, breathing, growing, diving, etc.).[15] Actions that take an object are encoded by transitive verbs, whereas those that take no object are expressed via intransitive verbs.

Intransitive verbs
edit

In Indo-European languages like English, all intransitive verbs behave similarly (‘They sneeze/breathe/dive/think’/etc.). In Keresan, actions that take no object are conceptualized in two distinct ways depending on how the initiator of the action is implicated. More active-like intransitive verbs (e.g. ‘to sneeze’) are coded through one set of morphemes, whereas actions conceptualized as involving the initiator at a lesser degree (e.g. ‘to believe’) are coded using a separate set of prefixes.

Degrees of involvement of the initiator in Keres[8]
Actions Intransitive verb type
More to write (-dyàatra), to steal as a thief (-chʼáwʼa), to have diarrhea (-ushchʼi),

to leave (-mi), to whistle (-srbiitsa), to sweat (-shdyuwàan’i)

Active
Less to believe (-hima), to be born (-dyá), to sleep (-bái),

to be afraid (-tyishu), to forget (-dyúmidruwi)

Inactive

Ideas expressed in Indo-European languages with adjectives are most often encoded by verbs in Keresan. That is, in Keresan one express the idea in the sentence ‘He is selfish’ by saying something along the lines of ‘He selfishes’. In such “actions”, the entity that is characterized by them is not implicated in the action directly (i.e. it's beyond their control), and thus belong in the Inactive intransitive category. The different sets of prefixes are shown below:

Intransitive Prefixes by Verb Type
Active intransive Inactive intransitive
Prefix Example Prefix Example
First s(i)- sudyàatra I write srk- srkuhima I believe
Second sr- srúuchʼáwʼa you steal kɨdr- kɨdrâidyá you were born
Third k- kashdyuwàanʼi s/he sweats dz- dzíibái he is sleeping
Transitive verbs
edit
Transitive verb - Indicative mood (-ukạchạ 'to see')
Direct object
Singular
Subject First ('me’) Second (‘you’) Third (‘her’/‘him’) Fourth
First

(‘I’)

- srà-ukạchạ sì-ukạchạ -
I see you I see her/him
Second

(‘you’)

dyù-ukạchạ - srù-ukạchạ
you see me you see her/him
Third

(‘she’/‘he’)

srgù-ukạchạ kudrù -ukạchạ g-ukạchạ gù-ukạchạ
s/he sees me s/he sees you s/he sees her/him s/he sees something
Fourth

(‘one’)

- dzì-ukạchạ -
one sees it

Aspect

edit

Aspect in Keresan is signalled by suffixes.

-ajanu 'to rain'
kájáni it rains
káajáni it is raining
kájásɨ it keeps raining
káajatú it rained

Time (tense) adverbials

edit

The category of tense is expressed in Keresan via adverbs that indicate when the action about which one is speaking took place.

Time adverbials in Acoma Keres[10]
Past Future
tsikʼínuma long ago kúsra tonight
háma once, formerly nacháma tomorrow
súwa yesterday naháayashi day after tomorrow

Lexicon

edit

New words are coined through a number of roots that are combined to pre-existing ones. Compounding is a common strategy for word building, although derivation also occurs.

Numerals

edit

The Keresan numeral system is a base 10 system. Numerals 11–19, as well as those between the multiple of tens, are formed by adding the word kʼátsi (/ kʼátsʰɪ / 'ten') followed by the word dzidra (/tsɪtʂa/ 'more'). Numerals 20 and above are formed by adding a multiplicative adverb (-wa or -ya) to the base number and the word kʼátsi.[8]

Western Keres
1 ísrkʼé 11 kʼátsi-írskʼá-dzidra 21 dyúya-kʼátsi-íisrkʼé-dzidra
2 dyúuwʼée 12 kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra 22 dyúya-kʼátsi-dyú-dzidra
3 chameʼée 13 kʼátsi-chami-dzidra 30 chamiya-kʼátsi
4 dyáana 14 kʼátsi-dyáana-dzidra 40 dyáanawa-kʼátsi
5 táam'a 15 kʼátsi-táamʼa-dzidra 50 táamʼawa-kʼátsi
6 shʼísa 16 kʼátsi-shchʼísa-dzidra 60 shchʼísawa-kʼátsi
7 mʼáiʼdyàana 17 kʼátsi-mʼáidyana-dzidra 70 mʼáidyanawa-kʼátsi
8 kukʼúmishu 18 kʼátsi-kukʼúmishu-dzidra 80 kukʼúmishuwa-kʼátsi
9 máyúkʼu 19 kʼátsi-máiyúkʼa-dzidra 90 máiyúkʼuwa-kʼátsi
10 kʼátsi 20 dyúwa-kʼátsi 100 kʼádzawa-kʼátsi

Loanwords from Spanish

edit

European colonizers arriving in the Southwest US brought with them material culture and concepts that were unknown to the peoples living in the area. Words for the new ideas introduced by Spaniards were often borrowed into Keres directly from Early Modern Spanish, and a large number of these persists in Modern Keresan.[11]

Semantic domain Modern Western Keres Modern Spanish English translation
Household items kamárîita, kuchâaru, kujûuna, méesa, mendâan, kuwêeta camarita, cuchara, colchón, mesa, ventana, cubeta (Mexico) bed, spoon, mattress, table, window (glass), bucket
Social structure gumbanêerụ, rái, murâatụ, merigâanạ, kumanirá, ninêeru compañero, rey, mulato, americano(a), comunidad, dinero workmate, king, black person, white person, community house, money
Food géesu, arûusị, kawé, kurántụ, mantạgîiyụ, mandêegạ queso, arroz, café, cilantro, mantequilla, manteca cheese, rice, coffee, cilantro, butter, lard/butter
Animal husbandry kawâayu, kanêeru, kujíinu, kurá, dûura, wáakạshị caballo, carnero, cochino, corral, toro, vaca horse, sheep, pen/corral, bull, cow
Religious concepts míisa, Háasus Kuríistị, nachạwêena, guréesima misa, Jesús Cristo, Noche Buena, Cuaresma mass, Jesus Christ, Christmas, Lent
Days of the week tamîikụ, rûunishị, mâatịsị, mérikụsị, sruwêewesị, yêenịsị, sâawaru domingo, lunes, martes, miércoles, jueves, viernes, sábado Sunday, Monday, Tuesday, Wednesday, Thursday, Friday, Saturday

Proto-language

edit
Proto-Keresan
Reconstruction ofKeresan languages

Selected Proto-Keresan reconstructions of plants, animals, and toponyms by Miller and Davis (1963)[16] are given below. The IPA transcription is based on the authors' description. Underlined consonants in reconstructions refer to uncertainties by the authors regarding aspiration (p. 312); these are shown as capitalized consonants in the IPA.

no. gloss Proto-Keresan IPA
10 wheat *ʔáṣánɪ */ʔáʂáni̥/
17 centipede *ʔíʔìˑdʸawa */ʔíʔìːcàwà/
19 cholla cactus *ʔiˑbánɪ */ʔiːpáni̥/
27 porcupine *ʔiˑṣ̍á */ʔiːʂʼá/
45 toad *bêˑrak̠ᴀ */pêːɾàKḁ/
63 turkey *cinᴀ */tsʰinḁ/
64 fox *cúsk̠ɪ */tsʰúsKi̥/
71 locust *c̍íˑga */tsʼíːkà/
72 Zia Pueblo *c̍íˑy̍á */tsʼíːjˀá/
78 kiva *c̆ídʸá */tʃícá/
83 medicine man *č̇áyâˑni */tʃʼájâːnì/
84 hawk *č̇ɨ́ˑríga */tʃʼɨ́ːɾíka/
85 horned toad *dabínᴜsk̠ᴀ */*tapínu̥sKḁ/
87 Santa Ana Pueblo *dámáyá */támájá/
88 squash *dâˑni */tâːnì/
91 corn husk *díˑskámí */tíːskʰámí/
93 dog *díyᴀ /*tíjḁ/
98 bobcat *dʸáˑdʸᴜ */cáːcu̥/
101 deer *dʸán̍é */cánˀé/
104 gourd *dʸáˑwí */cáːwí/
105 piñon pine *dʸèic̠ɪ */cèiTSi̥
108 elk *dʸɨ́ˑṣᴀ */cɨ́ːʂḁ/
110 badger *dʸúˑbí */*cúːpí/
112 beans *gánami */kánàmì/
114 seed *gáwɪc̠ɪ */káwi̥TSi̥/
119 bear *gúháyᴀ */kúhájḁ/
124 yucca *háʔásc̐á */háʔástʃʼá/
127 oak *ha̍ˑbánɪ
137 pine tree *hâˑniˑ */hâːniː/
147 Jemez Pueblo *héˑmíšíˑ-cɪ, *héˑmíšíˑ-zé /*héːmíʃíː-tsʰi̥/, */*héːmíʃíː-tsé/
149 turtle *héyᴀdʸɪ */héjḁci̥/
157 willow *híẓᵻsk̍áwa */*híʂɨ̥skʼáwa/
158 dove *húˑʔùˑga
161 yucca fruit *hùˑsk̍ani
169 antelope *kɨ́ˑc̠ɪ
175 wolf *k̍ákana
176 spider *k̍ámᴀsk̠ᵻ
198 mountain lion *mûˑk̍aiẓᴀ
200 buffalo *múšêiẓᴀ
201 soapweed *múšɪ
213 hummingbird *m̍îˑzᴀ
225 prairie dog *nɨ́t̠ɪ
232 bedbug *peséc̍uru
239 salamander *p̍águra
241 rabbit *rèˑdʸᴀ
246 woodpecker *sbíga
247 chicken *sbíˑná
251 meadowlark *sc̐áˑná
254 grasshopper *sc̐ár̍ɪ
260 crow *sc̐ɨ́r̍á
262 wild honey *sc̐úmᵻ
264 mosquito *sc̐úy̍úˑná
274 ant *síˑʔí
275 squirrel *síˑdʸᴀ
279 mouse *síyan̍ᵻ
282 bighorn sheep *skàˑsk̠ᴜ
286 bullsnake *sk̍áʔáˑdʸᴜ
287 fish *sk̍àˑšᵻ
291 peas *sk̍úrúˑná
293 dwarf corn *spíníní
306 parrot *šâˑwit̠ᴀ
307 flea, louse *šínaˑ
309 goose *šúˑdá
318 blue jay *ṣúisɪ
319 snake *ṣûˑwiˑ
342 abalone shell *w̍a̍ˑbɨ́nɪ
347 duck *w̍âˑyuṣᴀ
354 corn silk *yábášɪ
355 corn *yáˑčínɪ
356 mesquite *yêˑt̠ᴜ
357 worm *yúʔúbɨ́
369 corn cob *y̍úˑskúm̍á
edit

Keres was one of the seven languages sung in the Coca-Cola "It's Beautiful" commercial during the 2014 Super Bowl featuring "America the Beautiful".[17]

See also

edit

References

edit
  1. ^ "Detailed Languages Spoken at Home and Ability to Speak English for the Population 5 Years and over: 2009-2013".
  2. ^ "Keres". Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Merriam-Webster.
  3. ^ "Keres language, alphabet, and pronunciation". Omniglot. Retrieved April 28, 2021.
  4. ^ Ian., Maddieson (1984). Patterns of sounds. Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521113267. OCLC 10724704.
  5. ^ a b Davis, Irvine (1964). The Language of Santa Ana Pueblo, Smithsonian Bulletin 191, Anthropological Papers, No. 69.
  6. ^ a b A Comparative Sketch of Pueblo Languages: Phonology. Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics. 1987.
  7. ^ a b Spencer, Robert F. (1946). The Phonemes of Keresan.
  8. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Lachler, Jordan (2005). Grammar of Laguna Keres. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Dissertation.
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Valiquette, Hilaire (1990). A study for a lexicon of Laguna Keresan.
  10. ^ a b c d e Maring, Joel M. (1967). Grammar of Acoma Keresan. Indiana University Dissertation.
  11. ^ a b Spencer, Robert (1947). "Spanish Loanwords in Keresan". Southwestern Journal of Anthropology. 3 (2): 130–146. doi:10.1086/soutjanth.3.2.3628729. S2CID 164169397.
  12. ^ Brandt, Elizabeth (1981). "Native American Attitudes toward Literacy and Recording in the Southwest". Journal of the Linguistic Association of the Southwest. 4 (2): 185–195.
  13. ^ "The Keres Language Project". The Keres Language Project. Retrieved 2018-03-18.
  14. ^ L., Bybee, Joan (1994). The evolution of grammar : tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Perkins, Revere D. (Revere Dale), Pagliuca, William. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0226086631. OCLC 29387125.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  15. ^ Givón, Talmy (2001). Syntax : an introduction. Volume 1 (Rev. ed.). Amsterdam: J. Benjamins. ISBN 1588110656. OCLC 70727915.
  16. ^ Miller, Wick R.; Davis, Irvine (1963). "Proto-Keresan Phonology". International Journal of American Linguistics. 29 (4): 310–330. doi:10.1086/464748. S2CID 143519987.
  17. ^ "Native Language Spotlighted During Coca-Cola Super Bowl Ad". Indian Country Today Media Network. 2014-02-03. Archived from the original on 2014-03-01. Retrieved 2014-02-26.

Bibliography

edit
edit